组卷网 > 高中英语综合库 > 主题 > 人与社会 > 科普与现代技术 > 科普知识
题型:阅读理解-阅读单选 难度:0.65 引用次数:88 题号:12089160

Tests during criminal investigations (调查). But the test results are not always correct. When the police asked a person questions important to a criminal case, a fast heartbeat or sweaty hands might make a nervous truth-teller appear to be lying. And, an experienced liar might be able to control those signs and avoid suspicion (怀疑).

Our eyes look at a familiar face differently than they look at an unfamiliar one. When people look at unfamiliar faces, their eyes usually move from one feature, like the shape of the nose, to other features. Their eyes stop quickly as they try to identify the unknown person. When looking at a familiar face, people usually direct their attention to just a few features. But the researchers want to know if people could change those when they are hiding the truth.

The researchers showed 48 students pictures of both strangers and professors they knew. They asked all of the students to try to appear honest while they were lying about recognizing familiar faces. The researchers told some students to stop in the same areas when they were looking at both familiar and unfamiliar faces. Their eyes would start on the person’s forehead. Next, they looked at each eye, and then moved from one ear to the other, then down to the nose, mouth and chin.

In most cases, the students who tried to hide their recognition were not able to do so. When they looked at a familiar face, their eyes still had fewer stops in the inner parts of the face. Alisa Millen, a psychology researcher at the University of Stirling, said hiding markers for facial recognition in eye movements is difficult, especially if you know that person well. She added the more the students tried to hide recognition of a face, the more obvious it was. Millen hopes that the findings can someday be used in law enforcement (执法). She believes such knowledge could help investigators have a clearer understanding of who is connected to whom in a criminal case.

1. While looking at unfamiliar faces, our eyes usually ________.
A.view more facial features with stopsB.make indirect contact with them
C.notice familiar features firstD.focus on their eye movements
2. What method did the participants in the study use to hide their recognition?
A.To stop their eyes occasionally.
B.To fix their eyes on one facial feature.
C.To have longer stops on unfamiliar faces.
D.To observe facial features in a certain order.
3. What can we infer from Paragraph 4?
A.The students in the study are good liars.
B.The findings are widely used by the police.
C.It’s difficult for students to hide their true thoughts.
D.It’s reliable to see if someone is lying by their eye movements.
4. What is Alisa Millen’s attitude towards the application of the findings?
A.Positive.B.Doubtful.
C.Confused.D.Negative.

相似题推荐

阅读理解-阅读单选(约380词) | 适中 (0.65)
文章大意:本文是说明文。研究发现,慷慨使人更快乐、更健康。

【推荐1】In a study published in Nature Communications, researchers from the University of Zurich told 50 people they’d be receiving about $100. Half of the people were asked to spend that money on themselves, and half were asked to spend it on someone they knew. Before giving out any money, the researchers brought everyone into the lab and asked them to think about a friend they’d like to give a gift to and how much they would spend. They then performed functional MRI scans ( 扫描) to get to know activity in three parts of the brain related to social behavior, generosity, happiness and decision-making.

Their choices — and their brain activity — seemed to depend on how they had promised to spend the money earlier. Those who had promised to spend money on other people tended to make more generous decisions throughout the experiment ( 实验). They also had more interaction between the parts of the brain related to altruism ( 无私) and happiness, and they reported higher levels of happiness after the experiment was over.

Another piece of good news was that planning to give away just a little bit of money had the same effects on happiness as giving away a lot. “At least in our study, the amount spent did not matter,” says lead author Philippe Tobler. “Even little things have a beneficial effect — like bringing coffee to one’s workmates.”

It’s not yet clear how long these warm feelings last after being generous. But studies have shown that older people who are generous tend to have better health, says Tobler, and other research has suggested that spending money on others can be effective at lowering blood pressure. “Moreover, there is a positive connection between helping others and life expectancy (预期寿命) ,” he adds.

Next time you think that the best way to make yourself feel better is to buy yourself a treat, consider that the opposite is likely true. “It is worth giving it a try, even if you think it would not work,” Tobler says. “Repeated practice is probably needed so that giving becomes second nature.”

1. What did the researchers find out about those promising to spend money on other people?
A.They were much happier.
B.They had less brain activity.
C.They were slower at making decisions.
D.They experienced higher levels of stress.
2. What does Tobler want to explain by mentioning bringing coffee to workmates?
A.It isn’t hard to behave generously.
B.Being generous will pay off in the end.
C.The amount of generosity did not matter.
D.Small acts of generosity can be seen everywhere.
3. What does paragraph 4 mainly tell us?
A.Ways to show generosity.
B.Health benefits of being generous.
C.Reasons for performing acts of generosity.
D.Influences generous people have on others.
4. What does Tobler suggest people do in the last paragraph?
A.Practice giving and make it a habit.
B.Don’t expect anything in return when giving.
C.Don’t believe happiness depends on a good treat.
D.Perform something good even if it may have a bad result.
2023-05-26更新 | 29次组卷
阅读理解-阅读单选(约380词) | 适中 (0.65)
名校
文章大意:这是一篇说明文。文章主要说明了记忆是可塑的,容易改变,并通过介绍一些研究和实验论证了这一点。

【推荐2】What’s your first memory? Whatever it is, you’re bound to treasure it. But can we actually trust them? Nothing about memory is simple. Memory is malleable. What we remember is not necessarily what happened. A memory is not a recording. It’s more like a dramatic reconstruction and one that we can keep changing without realizing it.

For any experience to be remembered, it has to be encoded (编码). This encoding is not any kind of direct translation, though. It’s a rich and complex process that creates associations and meanings. We might be remembering something very similar, but slightly changed and colored by our own sets of associations.

Psychologist Elizabeth Loftus produced groundbreaking research on eyewitness testimony (证据). Her work showed that memories can easily be influenced, even after they’ve been created. For example, if two eyewitnesses talk with each other, their memories of events often change, absorbing what they’ve heard from the other one — but they won’t realize this has happened. Similarly, witnesses who are shown an image of an innocent person, can sometimes stick it onto their memory of the actual event — a process known as unconscious transference (无意识的转移). It is estimated that around 70% of wrongful convictions (定罪), later overturned by DNA evidence, are due to mistaken eyewitness testimony.

In some cases, memories can even be deliberately created and implanted. The Lost in the Mall experiment took a test group of subjects and talked to them in depth about key childhood memories while also adding an invented one — the experience of having been lost in a shopping center. It was found that between a quarter and a third of subjects not only accepted this new memory as genuine but enriched it with specific details.

Most of us have certain key memories of being a very young child. But research suggests that they’re highly unlikely to be actual memories due to the way memory is stored in the baby brain. Your precious first memory may well not be a real memory, and we’re all perhaps living in our imaginations more than we realize.

1. What does the underlined word “malleable” in paragraph 1 mean?
A.Trustworthy.B.False.C.Easily changed.D.Deeply impressive.
2. Which will influence the memory of eyewitnesses?
A.Powerful DNA evidence.B.Pictures stuck in their memory.
C.Reduced accuracy of convictions.D.Exchanged information between them.
3. What can we learn about “The Lost in the Mall” experiment?
A.It was designed to investigate mistaken eyewitness testimony.
B.Its subjects could recall specific details of a shopping center.
C.Its subjects made believe that they had been lost in a shopping center.
D.It demonstrated that invented experiences could be fixed in one’s memory.
4. What can be a suitable title for the text?
A.The Unreliable Nature of Memory.B.Revised Eyewitness Testimony.
C.Reflection on Sweet Childhood.D.An Exploration of Truth and Fiction.
2024-06-03更新 | 126次组卷
阅读理解-阅读单选(约370词) | 适中 (0.65)
名校

【推荐3】Have you ever caught yourself saying, "I hate small talk!" the minute you walk into a party? If so, you might be an introvert (内向的人). Introverts hate to chat about "small" subjects with people. They are fine when you talk one on one but seem to get lost in the crowd when there's a group around.

If you're talking to an introvert, you might find that your first few conversations go one of two ways. Either they'll seem to get bored and not say much or they'll seem to skate over small talk in favor of more in-depth topics. What you need to know is that the introvert is not being rude. It’s just that they don't like small talk. Here's why.

Small talk serves as a gateway toward deeper conversation. Some introverts don’t realize that small talk will actually help them get to the bigger topics. You don't need to stay on small talk forever. However, sometimes introverts feel that even the short amount of time it takes to get into the real conversation is a waste of time. Small talk is very fast. Someone says something and this should be followed up with a quick response. But introverts like to chew on their words before saying them aloud. Before they answer, they’ll mull over different thoughts.

Small talk by its very nature invites more people to join in. The more people who join the conversation, the more the introvert will tend to step back. As a result, they’ll be quiet. Small talk is about throwing out subjects in the hope that one or more of them can be turned into a common point of interest.

The listening involved with small talk is not in-depth. The purpose is about listening "lightly" so you can figure out the right topics to spend some time on. Introverts tend to be great listeners, but they use active listening rather than light listening. This makes them wonderful friends, but can also make it difficult for new people to understand why they’re so quiet.

1. How do introverts respond to a chat?
A.They try to avoid it.
B.They get excited about it.
C.They give it their full attention.
D.They focus on unimportant topics.
2. What do we know about small talk?
A.It goes slowly.
B.It costs people a lot of time.
C.It contributes to deeper topics.
D.It makes no sense.
3. What does the underlined phrase "mull over" in Paragraph 3 probably mean?
A.Give up.B.Think over.
C.Write down.D.Forget about.
4. How can we become a good talker in small conversations?
A.Figure out the deep meaning of questions.
B.Think twice before answering questions.
C.Try to find shared topics.
D.Be a quiet listener.
2020-09-11更新 | 128次组卷
共计 平均难度:一般